Case Study Piaget Object Performance

Using Object Play to Address Process Skills

Because Piagetian theory is the premier window on both infant cognitive development and play development, the relationship of play and cognition is deeply woven for therapists. The following discussion of using object play to facilitate cognition is based on the Piagetian stages found in the sensorimotor period (birth to 2 years). Although it is not possible to discuss Piagetian theory fully within this chapter, Piaget's work is recommended to all pediatric therapists who find themselves frequently addressing cognitive development.

The first stage (birth to 1 month) of the sensorimotor period is elementary sensorimotor adaptations. This stage is characterized by simple reflexes, uncoordinated movements, and the first discrimination and recognition of human and nonhuman objects. Selection of play objects for goals at this level of cognition strives simply to focus attention on an interesting object, produce reactions that demonstrate recognition, and provide experiences of reflexive grasp. Objects to attract visual gaze should offer visual contrast or high responsivity. For grasp, the objects must be of hand size.

The second stage (1 to 4 months) involves acquired adaptations. The first primary circular reactions, the child's retention of chance results in play, are seen in this stage. Object play facilitating cognition in the second stage uses visual observation of stationary and moving objects, early sound play turn taking, reflexive holding of an object and moving it within the visual field, and mouthing of objects in hand or not in hand. This is a stage at which a large variety of rattles, balls, and other small graspable objects are most useful.

Stage three (4 to 8 months) of the sensorimotor period, intentional sensorimotor adaptations, is dominated by the repetition of discovered patterns. The child begins distinguishing means and ends. Parts of an action are linked in time, but a series of actions is not. At this stage, objects that produce a clear direct response to the child's actions are useful, such as busy boxes, infant keyboards, and percussion toys. The child spends a long time simply grasping, handing from one hand to the other, and releasing objects, often until all objects within reach are beyond reach. If the infant is crawling, he or she pursues objects within close visual range. Discovering objects that are partially obscured leads to establishment of object permanence.

The fourth stage (8 to 12 months) involves further formation of concepts regarding the properties of objects. Time, causality, and space relationships begin to form as the child travels through the setting. The child begins to circumvent barriers to reach a desired object, further demonstrating emerging object permanence. Intermediary objects are used to reach goals, as in climbing on something else to get up on the couch, or pulling on a blanket to get a toy that is on it. The child more clearly anticipates outcomes from indicators in the environment. Play strategies from one toy are tried out on another toy. At this age objects begin to be used in simple combinations, such as banging a spoon on a tray and dumping things out of containers.

The fifth stage (12 to 18 months) is the stage of experimentation. Much time may be spent in what appears to be repetition but is actually subtly varied action. Large numbers of objects are typically engaged over time, yielding what can seem to the parent to be an explosion of toys, pots and pans, clothing, and other objects scattered through the house. Putting in, taking out, transporting, and early matching of parts (pans and lids often) are constant. Children's understanding of how objects can be combined becomes so much more sophisticated that by the end of this stage they begin simple tool use, such as drawing with chalk, stirring with a spoon, or other functional uses of one object on another.

The sixth stage (18 to 24 months) of the sensorimotor period is characterized by the use of mental combinations to invent new means of acting on objects. The child now encounters many of the “problems” of reaching an end and accommodates to them internally through mental combinations. This results primarily in a difference in speed of invention in comparison to the previous stage, as well as an abbreviation of object manipulations leading to invention. Object combinations become even more complex as the child begins simple constructions, continues to develop tool use, and enters fully into imitation of others as a mode of exploring objects.

The sensorimotor period is followed by the preoperational period, which lasts from 2 to 7 years of age. The preoperational period is characterized by the egocentrism of the child, demonstrated in attitudes of animism, realism, and magical thinking. Games of make-believe predominate from 2 to 4 years, giving way to interactive games with increasingly complex shared rules (Box 7-3).

Jean Piaget's Object Permanence: Theoretical Outline, Modern Critique and Parental Advice!

  • Get a thorough definition of Jean Piaget's theory of object permanence along with a description of his different stages of young children's cognitive development!


  • Also read about recent research's criticism of Jean Piaget's age specificity in his cognitive theories!


  • And finally find inspiration for games that you may play with your infant. Games that aim at stimulating and training your baby's cognitive development!


Connecting to Our Children

As adults we have all acquired the necessary skill of abstraction that is characteristic of object permanence.

We know that objects continue to exist even when we cannot see, hear, or touch them!

Understanding the fascinating process of intellectual and emotional development will help us to 'step into our children's shoes so to speak, and to see the world through their eyes and consequently help us connect to their needs.


Jean Piaget:
The Father of Object Permanence

Swiss psychologist, Jean Piaget, conducted studies to show that object permanence plays an important role in cognitive development.

As adults is interesting and almost incomprehensible to realize that young infants do not believe that objects exist separate from their actions or perceptions.

Young infants do not yet possess the ability to remember or even think about things they cannot see. And this ability actually goes beyond 'out of sight, out of mind' because to them, if they cannot see it, it simply does not exist.

In other words, their entire world is only what they can see at any given moment. Or to use a spiritual terminology, young infants live completely in the NOW. Past and future doesn't exist as conscious concepts.

This is why we may take a toy away from an infant and then infant will not react. The infant will not even cry or even try to look for what has disappeared. It's just gone as if it's never been there!


The Milestone of Object Permanence

Object permanence is a very important milestone in the social and intellectual development of our children, and as parents or caregivers, we can help our little ones progress through the series of stages in their thinking so that they gain a strong sense of trust and security.

Jean Piaget discovered that children actually construct their knowledge or ideas of the world based on what they perceive and what they experience.

The sensori-motor stage, from birth to the end of the 2nd year, is a time of rapid cognitive development and growth. Literally your baby's brain development is at constant full throttle!

More than any other period in life, these years are vital in the development of skills such as memory, imagining, creativity, deductive reasoning, and planning.

When children are able to create an image of an object or person in their mind, and then make decisions to act on that image, they have reached what is called full object permanence.


The Stages of Object Permanence

1) From 0-4 Months: Reflexes

During this stage babies learn about how their bodies move through repetition of reflexes.

As they grow, they will learn how to control these reflexes to help them adapt and function in their world.

Since action is mostly reflexive, infants at this stage are not really aware of objects to know that they have disappeared from sight.


2) From 0-4 Months: Primary Circular Reactions

Around 3 or 4 months babies will begin to notice objects and follow movements with their eyes.

If you show infants a ball or colorful toy, they will express delight; however, they will not react when the object is taken away or hidden.

At this stage of baby brain development, babies don't really care because they don't know that the toy still exists. s

They still give no thought to what they cannot see.


3) From 4-8 Months: Secondary Circular Reactions

Parents and caregivers will notice that children in this stage will begin to reach for objects, even when they are partially hidden.

This shows that they are beginning to gain an understanding of object permanence and realize that the whole exists even though they are only able to see a part.

However, if an object is completely hidden, they will make no attempt to look for it.

This is a fun stage since most infants actually try to grasp and play with toys.


4) From 8-12 Months: Co-ordination of Secondary Circular Reactions

At this stage, babies are able to retrieve a completely hidden object but will only look where they first saw the object hidden.

For example, infants will search for a hidden toy when they see it placed under a blanket.

However, if the toy is removed from under the blanket and re-hidden under a chair, the babies will still look under the blanket, even though they saw that the toy was hidden under a different cover.

This is an exciting progression because children now realize that an object exists outside of their perceptions.

If we show them a toy and then hide it, they know that not only does the toy still exist but they can actually actively begin searching for it.

They will begin their search where they last saw the object, and may even continue to familiar or expected places; however, if the toy does not reappear, they may quickly give up or even become frustrated and begin crying.

See an example of this stage in the video below:


5) From 12-18 Months: Tertiary Circular Reactions

At this stage, babies are able to find an object that is hidden, retrieved, and then re-hidden.

They enjoy playing games such as hide-and-seek and will even begin trying new actions to get different results.

Unlike the previous stage, they are able to account for changes in position that an object may make and adjust their actions accordingly.


5) From 18+ Months: Inventive and New Means Through Mental Combinations

This is just a fancy way of saying that infants in this stage fully understand object permanence.

They no longer interact with the world around them on a purely physical or motor skills level but are able to form internal pictures of objects that they have previously seen and understand that these objects still exist even when they are out of sight.

They are also able to account for the displacement of objects, meaning that they will look in multiple locations, even in 'hidden' areas and containers such as toy boxes, cupboards, or under furniture.

This stage is very important in cognitive development because it indicates that children not only realize the permanence of certain objects but are also able to see the world more objectively, knowing that it is not attached to themselves but actually exists independently of their perceptions and actions.


Recent Research Questions Jean Piaget's Theories as Underestimating Children's Mental Abilities?

While Jean Piaget's theory is solid and the progression through the stages is well documented, it is important to remember that every child is an individual.

Please be aware that theories and techniques are simply frameworks and guidelines that can help us understand our children, but should not be considered as bulletproof theories.

In fact, further studies on infant psychology have 'attacked' Piaget's timeline questioning whether Jean Piaget generally underestimates the rate and scope children's mental development:

  • Do children really not fully understand object permanence until they are 2 years old? Could the process begin earlier or finish later?


  • Do children younger that 4 months old have no concept at all of object permanence?


  • Do some children actually develop this concept much earlier but it does not manifest in their actions due to undeveloped motor skills or an inability to communicate?



Object Permanence May Happen Earlier Than Jean Piaget Suggests

Piaget has been criticized because his studies were limited only to the infant's visual perceptions or conceptualization of his or her world and failed to take into account information gained from other senses such as smell, taste, touch, or hearing.

Considering these factors, there has been some evidence that object permanence may actually develop earlier than Piaget indicated.

As parents most of us have probably experienced a fussy young infant that refuses to be placated until placed in his mother's arms. Or a crying baby that will be calmed simply by her parent's touch or smell. The concept of missing something and searching (or in this case, crying) for its return is an important aspect of object permanence.

If an infant younger than 4 months is incapable of thinking about things he cannot see, then it stands to reason that he would not be seeking a particular touch or smell. Yet, this is often the case. (For instance my own high need baby son developed baby separation anxiety much, much earlier than the norm which means that his sense of object permanence was far ahead than what Jean Piaget claims is the case.)

As parents, we can use Piaget's theory as a means of understanding the processes within our children, but we must also keep an open mind to the questions raised so that we do not confine our babies to popularly accepted norms and expectations.



Playing Object Permanence Games

As we now know infants build an understanding of their world through their motor abilities and information acquired from their senses such as touch, vision, taste, and movements.

Cognitive development is a result of interaction with the environments, so playing with our children can be a lot of fun during the sensori-motor stage.

By understanding what our children are capable of at each level of development, we can choose games and activities that will enhance their learning and include all of their senses.


Playing Peek-a-Boo

From approximately 6-12 months of age, this is supposedly one of the best activities to teach children that something or someone who cannot be seen still exists in their world.

For example covering a toy with a blanket then saying, “Where's the ball?” followed up by uncovering the toy and saying, “Here's the ball!” will help our children understand that the ball did not disappear just because the blanket covered it.

Hiding then reappearing from behind a door, playing with a Jack-in-the-box, or pulling objects from containers are also fun and effective ways to interact with our children while helping facilitate their intellectual and emotional development.


'Playing' Leave and Return

Whether it is simply leaving a room or leaving children with a babysitter and then reliably returning, this activity will help our children realize that even when they cannot see us, they know we will always come back.

A caregiver who is regularly out of sight yet consistently returns can actually encourage the development of object permanence as well as a strong sense of security.

Since many people suggest that object permanence involves all the senses, not simply visual perception, we can help solidify the idea by calling our children when we are away so that they hear our voices and realize that we still exist even though they cannot see us.

However, these 'games' might not appeal to all babies. For instance my own son wasn't at all interested in toys until quite late. The only thing that really interested him were human faces. So the above mentioned peek-a-boo games were of no good for me.

Once again babies are individuals just like adults so games and strategies are no one-size-fits-all!


Ensuring a Secure Environment

One of the best ways to help develop object permanence is to give young infants a secure, dependable, and loving environment in which to learn.

Babies it is important to introduce regular routines and familiar patterns so that they start to establish a sense of expectation.

If we always feed them in a certain way, read the same books to them daily, or place the same toys in their crib, they will begin to develop the concept of consistency and permanence as well as building both trust and security.

Complete object permanence is the ability to conjure up images in the mind and to use these images before taking action.

An important aspect in planning and decision making is the idea that expectations for the future are based on the past.

So, by giving our children reliable and stable environments we are encouraging object permanence and all the benefits this concept includes.

But once again not all babies are alike. For instance as an infant my son's stable environment was me, not the physical surroundings. So he would insist on always being on me while craving constant newness in his surroundings. In other words, he would be restless and bored (at least that was how I interpreted his signals) if I didn't often change physical locations!


Helping Our Infants Well Through the Milestone of Object Permanence

Object permanence is a big milestone in our children's development and understanding of their world.

The ability to create mental pictures or remember objects and people they have previously seen is a vital precursor to creativity and abstract thinking.

As parents, we can help our children become healthy, happy, and secure by understanding what they are experiencing at each stage of development, connecting with their feelings and emotions, and giving them what they need to realize that the people and things they love the most do not cease to exist just because they are out of sight.

Your Positive Parenting Ally,
Birgitte




Want to stay in touch and get the latest news?
Sign up for my free newsletter



0 Replies to “Case Study Piaget Object Performance”

Lascia un Commento

L'indirizzo email non verrà pubblicato. I campi obbligatori sono contrassegnati *